Neurosci & Educ–5 Days, 5 Ways. Day 1: Basic Architecture

Posted: 03 Dec 2012 02:14 AM PST

Neuroscience–especially human neuroscience, and more especially human functional brain imaging–has had a quite a run in the last twenty years. In the first decade the advances were known mostly to scientists. In the last ten years there have been plenty of articles in the popular press featuring brain images. Many of these articles have been breathless and silly. Some backlash was inevitable and one of the more potent examples was a recent op-ed in the New York Times. Still, as Gary Marcus pointed out in a nice blog piece, we would be wise not to throw the baby out with the bath water.

In that vein, I am following up on a piece I wrote last week, in which I argued that much of the work on this topic in education is neuro-garbage. Most of the piece was devoted to explaining why it’s difficult to apply neuroscience to education. (I left it to the reader to infer that it’s correspondingly easy to be glib.)

Picture

Toward the end of that piece I suggested that neuroscience can and has been usefully applied to problems in education. This week I’ll describe how. I’ll tackle one method each day this week.

I’ll keep things as simple as possible, but fasten your seatbelt if you feel the need.


Method 1: 

Neuroscience can give researchers clues about the basic architecture of a cognitive process. It can show that a cognitive process might be more complex than we would have otherwise guessed, or that it’s more simple.

Consider the figure below from Dehaene et al (2003) (click it for a larger version)

Picture

This figure summarizes a great deal of work indicating that there are three representations of number in the brain: a core quantity system (red), numbers in verbal form (green), and attentional orientation on the number line.

Suppose I am an educational psychologist, trying to figure out how children develop concepts of number, and how to coordinate the teaching of early mathematics with these concepts. I must have a theory of how number is represented in the mind. It’s possible–actually, it’s likely–that I would think of number as one thing, that children have one concept of the number five, for example. But this neuroscientific work indicates that the brain might use three representations of number. So it might be wise for me to use three representations in my cognitive theory of mathematics (which will support my educational theory).

In this example, there is greater diversity (three representations) where we might have guessed that we’d see simplicity (one representation). The opposite may also happen.

In one example, neuroscientific data were useful in interpreting variations in dyslexia across languages.

One of the peculiarities of dyslexia is that some key symptoms vary across different languages. For example, people with dyslexia usually show a large disparity between visual word recognition and IQ. But that disparity tends to be much larger in languages in which the spelling-sound correspondence is often inconsistent (e.g., English) than in languages where it’s more consistent (e.g., Italian).

This pattern raises the question: is what we’re calling “dyslexia” really the same thing in English and Italian? Maybe reading difficulties are so intertwined with the language you’re learning to read that it doesn’t make sense to call problems by the same label when they apply to English vs. Italian. Or maybe the problems kids develop in English-speaking vs. Italian-speaking countries is due to differences in the way reading tends to be taught in different countries.

Eraldo Paulesu and his colleagues (2000) used brain imaging data to argue that dyslexia is the same disorder in readers of different languages. They showed that the same brain region in left temporal cortex shows reduced activation during reading in French, Italian, and British readers who have been diagnosed with dyslexia.

Picture

Hence in this case neuroscientific data has shown us that there is simplicity (one reading problem) where we could have reasonably thought there was greater diversity (different reading problems across languages).

EDIT: It’s worth adding that anatomic separability (or overlap) doesn’t guarantee cognitive separability or identity. But it’s an indicator.

Tomorrow: Method 2.

References:
Dehaene, S., Pizaaz, M., Pinel, P., & Cohen, L. (2003). Three parietal circuits for number processing. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20,  487-506.

Paulesu, E., Demonet, J.-F., Fazio, F., McCrory, E., Chanoine, V., Brunswick, N., Cappa, S. F., Cossu, G., Habib, M., Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (2000). Dyslexia: Cultural diversity and biological unity. Science, 291, 2165-2167.

Acerca de mariluzarredondo

Secondary school English teacher

Responder

Introduce tus datos o haz clic en un icono para iniciar sesión:

Logo de WordPress.com

Estás comentando usando tu cuenta de WordPress.com. Cerrar sesión / Cambiar )

Imagen de Twitter

Estás comentando usando tu cuenta de Twitter. Cerrar sesión / Cambiar )

Foto de Facebook

Estás comentando usando tu cuenta de Facebook. Cerrar sesión / Cambiar )

Google+ photo

Estás comentando usando tu cuenta de Google+. Cerrar sesión / Cambiar )

Conectando a %s